Preprint / Version 1

A Kinarm Approach Avoidance Task

A robot-based reaching task to assess automatic attitudes towards visual stimuli

##article.authors##

  • Kayne Park University of Ottawa
  • Matthieu P. Boisgontier

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.51224/SRXIV.446

Keywords:

approach-avoidance, robotics, reaction time, speed, automatic processes, Psychology

Abstract

The investigation of automatic approach-avoidance attitudes has traditionally been conducted using computer-based technologies. These devices can characterize important measures of psychological function such as reaction time and error rates. However, they are unable to accurately quantify other crucial measures of human behaviour such as hand speed and movement direction. Novel robotic devices have been developed to allow highly quantitative measures of human motion. The Kinarm is an augmented reality device which can track a participant’s upper limb movement while they interact in novel environments. Here, we describe the adaptation of the commonly used Approach-Avoidance Task on the Kinarm Endpoint Laboratory. This variant of the task can assess movements of both arms and across many directions of reach. In addition, the Kinarm can provide resistive loads which can investigate the role of metabolic cost during approach or avoidance or be used for rehabilitation protocols.

Metrics

Metrics Loading ...

References

Allen, L., O’Connell, A., & Kiermer, V. (2019). How can we ensure visibility and diversity in research contributions? How the Contributor Role Taxonomy (CRediT) is helping the shift from authorship to contributorship. Learned Publishing, 32(1), 71–74. https://doi.org/10.1002/leap.1210

Barton, T., Constable, M. D., Sparks, S., & Kritikos, A. (2021). Self-bias effect: Movement initiation to self-owned property is speeded for both approach and avoidance actions. Psychological Research, 85(4), 1391–1406. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01325-0

Boisgontier, M. (2022). Research integrity requires to be aware of good and questionable research practices. European Rehabilitation Journal, 2(1), Article 1. https://doi.org/10.52057/erj.v2i1.24

Bourke, T. C., Lowrey, C. R., Dukelow, S. P., Bagg, S. D., Norman, K. E., & Scott, S. H. (2016). A robot-based behavioural task to quantify impairments in rapid motor decisions and actions after stroke. Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation, 13, 91. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12984-016-0201-2

Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., & Berntson, G. G. (1993). Rudimentary determinants of attitudes: II. Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(1), 5–17. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.1.5

Cheval, B., Daou, M., Cabral, D. A. R., Bacelar, M. F. B., Parma, J. O., Forestier, C., Orsholits, D., Sander, D., Boisgontier, M. P., & Miller, M. W. (2020). Higher inhibitory control is required to escape the innate attraction to effort minimization. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 51, 101781. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2020.101781

Cheval, B., Sarrazin, P., & Pelletier, L. (2014). Impulsive Approach Tendencies towards Physical Activity and Sedentary Behaviors, but Not Reflective Intentions, Prospectively Predict Non-Exercise Activity Thermogenesis. PLOS ONE, 9(12), e115238. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115238

Cheval, B., Tipura, E., Burra, N., Frossard, J., Chanal, J., Orsholits, D., Radel, R., & Boisgontier, M. P. (2018). Avoiding sedentary behaviors requires more cortical resources than avoiding physical activity: An EEG study. Neuropsychologia, 119, 68–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.07.029

Farajzadeh, A., Dionne, N., Benoit, N., Bezeau, O., Bourgie, T., Gerro, B., Ouimet, J., & Boisgontier, M. P. (2024). Apathy, Intentions, and Affective Attitudes in Physical Activity Behavior (p. 2024.07.16.24310493). medRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.07.16.24310493

Farajzadeh, A., Goubran, M., Beehler, A., Cherkaoui, N., Morrison, P., de Chanaleilles, M., Maltagliati, S., Cheval, B., Miller, M. W., Sheehy, L., Bilodeau, M., Orsholits, D., & Boisgontier, M. P. (2023). Automatic approach-avoidance tendency toward physical activity, sedentary, and neutral stimuli as a function of age, explicit affective attitude, and intention to be active. Peer Community Journal, 3. https://doi.org/10.24072/pcjournal.246

Gaprielian, P., Scott, S. H., & Levy, R. (2022). Reverse Visually Guided Reaching in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease. Parkinson’s Disease, 2022(1), 8132923. https://doi.org/10.1155/2022/8132923

Kemps, E., Tiggemann, M., Martin, R., & Elliott, M. (2013). Implicit approach–avoidance associations for craved food cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 19(1), 30–38. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031626

Krieglmeyer, R., & Deutsch, R. (2010). Comparing measures of approach–avoidance behaviour: The manikin task vs. two versions of the joystick task. Cognition and Emotion, 24(5), 810–828. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930903047298

Lender, A., Meule, A., Rinck, M., Brockmeyer, T., & Blechert, J. (2018). Measurement of food-related approach-avoidance biases: Larger biases when food stimuli are task relevant. Appetite, 125, 42–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2018.01.032

Lowrey, C. R., Dukelow, S. P., Bagg, S. D., Ritsma, B., & Scott, S. H. (2022). Impairments in Cognitive Control Using a Reverse Visually Guided Reaching Task Following Stroke. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 36(7), 449–460. https://doi.org/10.1177/15459683221100510

Marsh, A. A., Ambady, N., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). The Effects of Fear and Anger Facial Expressions on Approach- and Avoidance-Related Behaviors. Emotion, 5(1), 119–124. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.119

Marteau, T. M., Hollands, G. J., & Fletcher, P. C. (2012). Changing Human Behavior to Prevent Disease: The Importance of Targeting Automatic Processes. Science, 337(6101), 1492–1495. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1226918

Phaf, R. H., Mohr, S. E., Rotteveel, M., & Wicherts, J. M. (2014). Approach, avoidance, and affect: A meta-analysis of approach-avoidance tendencies in manual reaction time tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00378

Roy, N., Karnick, H., & Verma, A. (2023). Towards the self and away from the others: Evidence for self-prioritization observed in an approach avoidance task. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1041157. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1041157

Scott, S. H. (1999). Apparatus for measuring and perturbing shoulder and elbow joint positions and torques during reaching. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 89(2), 119–127. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0270(99)00053-9

Simmatis, L. E. R., Jin, A. Y., Keiski, M., Lomax, L. B., Scott, S. H., & Winston, G. P. (2020). Assessing various sensorimotor and cognitive functions in people with epilepsy is feasible with robotics. Epilepsy & Behavior: E&B, 103(Pt A), 106859. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2019.106859

Solarz, A. K. (1960). Latency of instrumental responses as a function of compatibility with the meaning of eliciting verbal signs. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(4), 239–245. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0047274

Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., Tibboel, H., Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., Verbanck, P., Hanak, C., Brevers, D., & Noël, X. (2013). On the predictive validity of automatically activated approach/avoidance tendencies in abstaining alcohol-dependent patients. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 127(1), 81–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.06.019

Vanderlinden, J. A., Semrau, J. S., Silver, S. A., Holden, R. M., Scott, S. H., & Boyd, J. G. (2022). Acute kidney injury is associated with subtle but quantifiable neurocognitive impairments. Nephrology Dialysis Transplantation, 37(2), 285–297. https://doi.org/10.1093/ndt/gfab161

Wiers, R. W., Rinck, M., Kordts, R., Houben, K., & Strack, F. (2010). Retraining automatic action-tendencies to approach alcohol in hazardous drinkers. Addiction, 105(2), 279–287. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02775.x

Wittekind, C. E., Blechert, J., Schiebel, T., Lender, A., Kahveci, S., & Kühn, S. (2021). Comparison of different response devices to assess behavioral tendencies towards chocolate in the approach-avoidance task. Appetite, 165, 105294. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2021.105294

Wittekind, C. E., Feist, A., Schneider, B. C., Moritz, S., & Fritzsche, A. (2015). The approach-avoidance task as an online intervention in cigarette smoking: A pilot study. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 46, 115–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.08.006

Downloads

Posted

2024-09-07