Preprint / Version 1

Why people should run after positive affective experiences, not health benefits

##article.authors##

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.51224/SRXIV.164

Keywords:

physical activity, decision making, health, affect, effort

Abstract

Most individuals are now aware of health benefits of physical activity (PA) but remain physically inactive. Mobilizing a multidisciplinary approach at the crossroads between decision-making sciences, we investigate why highlighting the health benefits of PA is unlikely to promote a sustained engagement in PA. Essential features of decision making – effort-discounting, delay-discounting and beliefs distortion – may weaken the subjective value attributed to health benefits, making the latter insufficient to trigger PA behaviors. We develop a decision model demonstrating that health benefits hold a weak subjective value, in comparison with the cost of engaging in PA (e.g., effort) and of our innate attraction toward sedentary alternatives. Instead, focusing on positive affective experiences could counteract the impact of aforementioned features and ultimately favor a regular engagement in PA. Tackling the current pandemic of physical inactivity would therefore require an urgent change in the promotion of PA, so as to make affective experiences central.

Metrics

Metrics Loading ...

References

Pontzer H. Ecological Energetics in Early Homo. Curr Anthropol. 2012;53(S6):S346-S358. doi:10.1086/667402

Kohl HW, Craig CL, Lambert EV, et al. The pandemic of physical inactivity: Global action for public health. Lancet. 2012. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60898-8

Bull FC, Al-Ansari SS, Biddle S, et al. World Health Organization 2020 guidelines on physical activity and sedentary behaviour. Br J Sports Med. 2020;54(24):1451-1462. doi:10.1136/bjsports-2020-102955

Bauman A, Chau J. The Role of Media in Promoting Physical Activity. J Phys Act Heal. 2009;6(s2):S196-S210. doi:10.1123/jpah.6.s2.s196

Fredriksson SV, Alley SJ, Rebar AL, Hayman M, Vandelanotte C, Schoeppe S. How are different levels of knowledge about physical activity associated with physical activity behaviour in Australian adults? PLoS One. 2018;13(11):e0207003. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0207003

Canadian Fitness and Lifestyle Research Institute. Physical activity monitor: facts and figures.

Rhodes RE, de Bruijn G-J. How big is the physical activity intention-behaviour gap? A meta-analysis using the action control framework. Br J Health Psychol. 2013;18(2):296-309. doi:10.1111/bjhp.12032

Bricca A, Harris LK, Jäger M, Smith SM, Juhl CB, Skou ST. Benefits and harms of exercise therapy in people with multimorbidity: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Ageing Res Rev. 2020;63:101166. doi:10.1016/j.arr.2020.101166

Saunders TJ, Dechman G, Hernandez P, et al. Distinct Trajectories of Physical Activity Among Patients with COPD During and After Pulmonary Rehabilitation. J Chronic Obstr Pulm Dis. 2015;12(5):539-545. doi:10.3109/15412555.2014.995286

Ekkekakis P. People have feelings! Exercise psychology in paradigmatic transition. Curr Opin Psychol. 2017;16:84-88.

Segar ML, Richardson CR. Prescribing Pleasure and Meaning. Am J Prev Med. 2014;47(6):838-841. doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2014.07.001

Rangel A, Camerer C, Montague PR. A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(7):545-556. doi:10.1038/nrn2357

Rangel A, Hare T. Neural computations associated with goal-directed choice. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2010;20(2):262-270. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2010.03.001

Floresco SB, Onge JR St., Ghods-Sharifi S, Winstanley CA. Cortico-limbic-striatal circuits subserving different forms of cost-benefit decision making. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2008;8(4):375-389. doi:10.3758/cabn.8.4.375

Basten U, Biele G, Heekeren HR, Fiebach CJ. How the brain integrates costs and benefits during decision making. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2010;107(50):21767-21772. doi:10.1073/pnas.0908104107

Prévost C, Pessiglione M, Météreau E, Cléry-Melin M-L, Dreher J-C. Separate valuation subsystems for delay and effort decision costs. J Neurosci. 2010;30(42):14080-14090.

Hagura N, Haggard P, Diedrichsen J. Perceptual decisions are biased by the cost to act. Elife. 2017;6. doi:10.7554/elife.18422

Reimers CD, Knapp G, Reimers AK. Does Physical Activity Increase Life Expectancy? A Review of the Literature. J Aging Res. 2012;2012:1-9. doi:10.1155/2012/243958

Ainslie G. Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychol Bull. 1975;82(4):463-496. doi:10.1037/h0076860

Rachlin H, Raineri A, Cross D. Subjective probability and delay. J Exp Anal Behav. 1991;55(2):233-244. doi:10.1901/jeab.1991.55-233

Rick S, Loewenstein G. Intangibility in intertemporal choice. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2008;363(1511):3813-3824. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0150

Lebreton M, Bertoux M, Boutet C, et al. A critical role for the hippocampus in the valuation of imagined outcomes. PLoS Biol. 2013;11(10). doi:10.1371/JOURNAL.pbio.1001684

Blackburn M, Mason L, Hoeksma M, Zandstra EH, El-Deredy W. Delay discounting as emotional processing: An electrophysiological study. Cogn Emot. 2012;26(8):1459-1474. doi:10.1080/02699931.2012.673478

Sharot T, Kanai R, Marston D, Korn CW, Rees G, Dolan RJ. Selectively altering belief formation in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2012;109(42):17058-17062. doi:10.1073/pnas.1205828109

Brownstein AL. Biased predecision processing. Psychol Bull. 2003;129(4):545-568. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.4.545

Weinstein ND. Unrealistic optimism about future life events. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;39(5):806-820. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.39.5.806

Ditto PH, Lopez DF. Motivated skepticism: Use of differential decision criteria for preferred and nonpreferred conclusions. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1992;63(4):568-584. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.568

Miller DT, Ross M. Self-serving biases in the attribution of causality: Fact or fiction? Psychol Bull. 1975;82(2):213-225. doi:10.1037/h0076486

Salander P. Attributions of lung cancer: my own illness is hardly caused by smoking. Psychooncology. 2007;16(6):587-592. doi:10.1002/pon.1121

Fox CR, Poldrack RA. Prospect Theory and the Brain. In: Neuroeconomics. Elsevier; 2014:533-567. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-416008-8.00042-5

Dukes D, Abrams K, Adolphs R, et al. The rise of affectivism. Nat Hum Behav. 2021;5(7):816-820. doi:10.1038/s41562-021-01130-8

Brand R, Ekkekakis P. Affective–Reflective Theory of physical inactivity and exercise: Foundations and preliminary evidence. Ger J Exerc Sport Res. 2018;48(1):48-58. doi:10.1007/s12662-017-0477-9

Conroy DE, Berry TR. Automatic Affective Evaluations of Physical Activity: Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2017;45(4):230-237. doi:10.1249/JES.0000000000000120

Cheval B, Boisgontier MP. The Theory of Effort Minimization in Physical Activity. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2021;49(3):168-178. doi:10.1249/JES.0000000000000252

Williams DM, Bohlen LC. Motivation for exercise: Reflective desire versus hedonic dread. In: APA Handbook of Sport and Exercise Psychology, Volume 2: Exercise Psychology (Vol. 2). Washington: American Psychological Association; 2019:363-385. doi:10.1037/0000124-019

Swann C, Jackman PC, Schweickle MJ, Vella SA. Optimal experiences in exercise: A qualitative investigation of flow and clutch states. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2019;40:87-98. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.09.007

Taylor IM, Smith K, Hunte R. Motivational processes during physical endurance tasks. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2020;30(9):1769-1776. doi:10.1111/sms.13739

Ekkekakis P. The Dual-Mode Theory of affective responses to exercise in metatheoretical context: II. Bodiless heads, ethereal cognitive schemata, and other improbable dualistic creatures, exercising. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol. 2009;2(2):139-160. doi:10.1080/17509840902829323

Liao Y, Shonkoff ET, Dunton GF. The Acute Relationships Between Affect, Physical Feeling States, and Physical Activity in Daily Life: A Review of Current Evidence. Front Psychol. 2015;6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01975

Reed J, Ones DS. The effect of acute aerobic exercise on positive activated affect: A meta-analysis. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2006;7(5):477-514. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2005.11.003

Ensari I, Greenlee TA, Motl RW, Petruzzello SJ. Meta-analysis of acute exercise effects on state anxiety: an update of randomized controlled trials over the past 25 years. Depress Anxiety. 2015;32(8):624-634. doi:10.1002/da.22370

White RL, Parker PD, Lubans DR, et al. Domain-specific physical activity and affective wellbeing among adolescents: an observational study of the moderating roles of autonomous and controlled motivation. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2018;15(1):87. doi:10.1186/s12966-018-0722-0

Ekkekakis P, Parfitt G, Petruzzello SJ. The pleasure and displeasure people feel when they exercise at different intensities. Sport Med. 2011;41(8):641-671.

Bourke M, Hilland TA, Craike M. Variance in the valenced response during moderate-to-vigorous physical activity: a review of cognitive and contextual mechanisms. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol. 2021;14(1):154-185. doi:10.1080/1750984X.2020.1780626

Lahart I, Darcy P, Gidlow C, Calogiuri G. The Effects of Green Exercise on Physical and Mental Wellbeing: A Systematic Review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(8):1352. doi:10.3390/ijerph16081352

Williams DM, Dunsiger S, Emerson JA, Gwaltney CJ, Monti PM, Miranda R. Self-Paced Exercise, Affective Response, and Exercise Adherence: A Preliminary Investigation Using Ecological Momentary Assessment. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2016;38(3):282-291. doi:10.1123/jsep.2015-0232

Zenko Z, Ekkekakis P, Ariely D. Can You Have Your Vigorous Exercise and Enjoy It Too? Ramping Intensity Down Increases Postexercise, Remembered, and Forecasted Pleasure. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2016;38(2):149-159. doi:10.1123/jsep.2015-0286

Zenko Z, Ekkekakis P, Kavetsos G. Changing minds: Bounded rationality and heuristic processes in exercise-related judgments and choices. Sport Exerc Perform Psychol. 2016;5(4):337-351. doi:10.1037/spy0000069

Downloads

Posted

2022-06-07